Science is the key …
Back in February, I wrote about a National Academies’ workshop that featured some outstanding members of the scientific and statistical world, and which gave participants to identify new potential subjects for inclusion in a proposed fourth edition of the Reference Manual on Scientific Evidence.[1] Funding for that new edition is now secured, and the National Academies has published a précis of the February workshop. National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine, Emerging Areas of Science, Engineering, and Medicine for the Courts: Proceedings of a Workshop – in Brief (Washington, DC 2021). The Rapporteurs for these proceedings provide a helpful overview for this meeting, which was not generally covered in the legal media.[2]
The goal of the workshop, which was supported by a planning committee, the Committee on Science, Technology, and Law, the National Academies, the Federal Judicial Center, and the National Science Foundation, was, of course, to identify chapters for a new, fourth edition, of Reference Manual on Scientific Evidence. The workshop was co-chaired by Dr. Thomas D. Albright, of the Salk Institute for Biological Studies, and the Hon. Kathleen McDonald O’Malley, Judge on the U.S. Court of Appeals for the Federal Circuit.
The Rapporteurs duly noted Judge O’Malley’s Workshop comments that she hoped that the reconsideration of the Reference Manual can help close the gap between science and the law. It is thus encouraging that the Rapporteurs focused a large part of their summary on the presentation of Professor Xiao-Li Meng[3] on selection bias, which “can come from cherry picking data, which alters the strength of the evidence.” Meng identified the
“7 S’(ins)” of selection bias:
(1) selection of target/hypothesis (e.g., subgroup analysis);
(2) selection of data (e.g., deleting ‘outliers’ or using only ‘complete cases’);
(3) selection of methodologies (e.g., choosing tests to pass the goodness-of-fit); (4) selective due diligence and debugging (e.g., triple checking only when the outcome seems undesirable);
(5) selection of publications (e.g., only when p-value <0.05);
(6) selections in reporting/summary (e.g., suppressing caveats); and
(7) selections in understanding and interpretation (e.g., our preference for deterministic, ‘common sense’ interpretation).”
Meng also addressed the problem of analyzing subgroup findings after not finding an association in the full sample, dubious algorithms, selection bias in publishing “splashy” and nominally “statistically significant” results, and media bias and incompetence in disseminating study results. Meng discussed how these biases could affect the accuracy of research findings, and how these biases obviously affect the accuracy, validity, and reliability of research findings that are relied upon by expert witnesses in court cases.
The Rapporteurs’ emphasis on Professor Meng’s presentation was noteworthy because the current edition of the Reference Manual is generally lacking in a serious exploration of systematic bias and confounding. To be sure, the concepts are superficially addressed in the Manual’s chapter on epidemiology, but in a way that has allowed many district judges to shrug off serious questions of invalidity with the shibboleth that such questions “to to the weight, not the admissibility,” of challenged expert witness opinion testimony. Perhaps the pending revision to Rule 702 will help improve fidelity to the spirit and text of Rule 702.
Questions of bias and noise have come to receive more attention in the professional statistical and epidemiologic literature. In 2009, Professor Timothy Lash published an important book-length treatment of quantitative bias analysis.[4] Last year, statistician David Hand published a comprehensive, but readily understandable, book on “Dark Data,” and the ways statistical and scientific interference are derailed.[5] One of the presenters at the February workshop, nobel laureate, Daniel Kahneman, published a book on “noise,” just a few weeks ago.[6]
David Hand’s book, Dark Data, (Chapter 10) sets out a useful taxonomy of the ways that data can be subverted by what the consumers of data do not know. The taxonomy would provide a useful organizational map for a new chapter of the Reference Manual:
A Taxonomy of Dark Data
Type 1: Data We Know Are Missing
Type 2: Data We Don’t Know Are Missing
Type 3: Choosing Just Some Cases
Type 4: Self- Selection
Type 5: Missing What Matters
Type 7: Changes with Time
Type 8: Definitions of Data
Type 9: Summaries of Data
Type 11: Feedback and Gaming
Type 12: Information Asymmetry
Type 13: Intentionally Darkened Data
Type 14: Fabricated and Synthetic Data
Type 15: Extrapolating beyond Your Data
Providing guidance not only on “how we know,” but also on how we go astray, patho-epistemology, would be helpful for judges and lawyers. Hand’s book really just a beginning to helping gatekeepers appreciate how superficially plausible health-effects claims are invalidated by the data relied upon by proffered expert witnesses.
* * * * * * * * * * * *
“There ain’t no room for the hopeless sinner
Who would hurt all mankind, just to save his own, believe me now
Have pity on those whose chances grow thinner”
[1] “Reference Manual on Scientific Evidence v4.0” (Feb. 28, 2021).
[2] Steven Kendall, Joe S. Cecil, Jason A. Cantone, Meghan Dunn, and Aaron Wolf.
[3] Prof. Meng is the Whipple V. N. Jones Professor of Statistics, in Harvard University. (“Seeking simplicity in statistics, complexity in wine, and everything else in fortune cookies.”)
[4] Timothy L. Lash, Matthew P. Fox, and Aliza K. Fink, Applying Quantitative Bias Analysis to Epidemiologic Data (2009).
[5] David J. Hand, Dark data : why what you don’t know matters (2020).
[6] Daniel Kahneman, Olivier Sibony, and Cass R. Sunstein, Noise: A Flaw in Human Judgment (2021).